The publication of Origin of Species
by Charles Darwin in 1859 initiated the beginning of a scientific, cultural and
social revolution - one that caused science and society to view the origin of
humankind and its relationships with other living beings differently. Not
surprisingly, Darwin's ideas have often been compared with those of Copernicus
because of their deep impact, not just in science, but in culture and human
understanding. As is the nature of science, ideas also change and evolve, and
the core of evolutionary thinking since the middle of the 20th century (the
so-called New or Modern Synthesis) is actually a mixture of Darwinian ideas
about natural selection and contemporary genetic theory, as initially developed
by people such as Dobzhansky [1937], Mayr [1942] and Simpson [1944]. Yet, the
principal mechanism of evolutionary change proposed by Darwin remains alive,
namely natural selection, which involves four straightforward principles: (1)
there are more members of a species born in each generation than will survive
(i.e., superfecundity); (2) there is variation in physical and behavioral
characteristics among individuals within species; (3) this variation is
heritable, and (4) characteristics that result in an individual surviving and
reproducing tend to increase in frequency in the population, whereas
characteristics of nonsurvivors decrease. Evidence from a broad range of
disciplines, including biogeography, paleontology, comparative anatomy,
embryology and molecular biology [Ayala, 1999; Weiner, 1995; US National
Academy of Sciences, 1984] has provided consistent support for Darwin's
contention, making the 'fact' of evolution undeniable, with natural selection
being the most potent (though likely not the sole) mechanism responsible for
changes in species over time [Gould, 2002; Mayr, 1982].
Psychological science as a whole -
and particularly developmental psychology has
not been immune from the findings and theories related to biological evolution.
In fact, the origin of developmental psychology
as a discipline has often been linked with the evolutionary thinking of the
scientific community during the latter half of the 19th century [Delval, 1982,
1988, 1994; Cairns, 1983; Dixon and Lerner, 1992]. However, others have
questioned the importance of such a link [Bradley, 1989; Charlesworth, 1992] or
proposed that developmental psychologists have
misunderstood Darwin's message, resulting in what Morss [1990] called the
'Darwinian myth', or what Lickliter and Berry [1990] referred to as the
'phylogeny fallacy'. Moreover, the main theoretical streams that have led
psychological thinking over the last century (behaviorism and cognitive psychology) have focused on levels of analyses that
did not make completely necessary reference to either biology or evolution
[Hernandez Blasi, 2000], and this includes mainstream developmental
psychology. To date, after almost 150 years since Darwin's ideas were
published, the influence of evolutionary ideas on developmental
psychological theory and research has been weak (although there are some
exceptions, for instance, attachment theory, see Bowlby [1969]). Evolutionary
thinking has remained within our discipline more as a vague theoretical
reference than as an active pool of ideas to serve as a framework for doing
research on development. As Charlesworth noted in 1992, 'the revolutionary
implications for studying human behavior from an evolutionary point of view are
just starting to be realized' (p. 9), and we believe that developmental psychology should partake fully in
21st century Zeitgeist that recognizes phylogenetic influences on human thought
and behavior.
Although few contemporary
psychologists, developmental or otherwise,
deny the fact of human evolution, there has been (and continues to be) a
long-standing resistance in the social and behavioral sciences to the
application of evolutionary theory to explain human behavior [Brown, 1991;
Geertz, 1973; see Tooby and Cosmides, 1992, for a review]. This stems, in part,
from a belief that humans possess a unique set of mental faculties that
provides them a degree of intentional control over their behavior, freeing them
from the constraints of biology and obviating a need for an understanding of
Homo sapiens' phylogenetic history in order to understand its behavior.
Relatedly, evolutionary psychology and other
biologically based approaches to human behavior smack of genetic determinism
and serve, in the minds of some, to minimize the role of culture in influencing
behavior. According to Tooby and Cosmides [1992], the standard social science
model, which proposed that humans possess no 'nature' but are solely the
products of their culture, dominated thinking in the social sciences through
the 20th century and made no room for evolutionary thought in explaining human
behavior.
Tooby and Cosmides [1992] discussed
at great lengths many of the shortcomings of the standard social science model,
and research over the past several decades has clearly shown that humans are
not born as blank slates but have biologically influenced constraints that
affect their perception, cognition and social and emotional behavior [Bjorklund
and Pellegrini, 2002; Gelman and Williams, 1998]. Moreover, contemporary
evolutionary psychological theory emphasizes that there is substantial
plasticity in development, making any claims of genetic determinism incorrect
(see further discussion below). Finally, an explosion of research in
comparative psychology has revealed both many
similarities and differences in the social and cognitive abilities of H.
sapiens and our great ape relatives [Byrne, 1995; Tomasello and Call, 1997;
Parker and McKinney, 1999; Suddenhof and Whiten, 2001], supporting the
existence of a phylogenetic continuity of mental function. Although such
discoveries and insights do not require that contemporary human functioning and
development be examined from an evolutionary perspective, we believe that such
advances make the time ripe for such an analysis.
0 komentar:
Posting Komentar